Strona Mirosława Dakowskiego. - A Scientific Scandal</td></tr><tr><td width="70%" align="left" valign="top" colspan="2"> <span class="small"> Wpisał: David Berlinski </span>    </td> </tr> <tr> <td valign="top" colspan="2" class="createdate"> 31.07.2007. </tr> <tr> <td valign="top" colspan="2"> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">David Berlinski</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt"><span>            </span>A Scientific Scandal</span></strong></p> <p style="margin: 0.25pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 1.45pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">In <span style="font-variant: small-caps">science, </span>as in life, it is always an excellent idea </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">to cut the cards after the deck has been shuf­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">fled. One may admire the dealer, but trust is an­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">other matter.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">In a recent essay in <span style="font-variant: small-caps">commentary, </span>"Has Darwin </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Met His Match?" (December 2002), I discussed, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">evaluated, and criticized theories of intelligent de­sign, which have presented the latest challenge to </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">Darwin</span><span style="font-size: 12pt">'s theory of evolution. In the course of the <span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">discussion I observed that <strong>the evolution of the </strong></span><strong><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">mammalian eye has always seemed difficult to </span>imagine.</strong> It is an issue that Darwin himself raised, <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">and although he settled the matter to his own satis­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">faction, biologists have long wished for a <em>direct </em></span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">demonstration that something like a functional eye </span>could be formed in reasonable periods of time by <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">means of the Darwinian principles of random vari­</span>ation and natural </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">selection.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">Just such a demonstration, I noted in my essay, is what the biologists Dan-Erik Nilsson and</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.2pt"> Su­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt">sanne Pelger seemed to provide in a 1994 paper. <em>[<span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">A Pessimistic Estimate of the Time Required for an Eye to </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Evolve," <span>Proceedings of the Royal Society, </span></span></em></span><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">London</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.05pt"> B (1994) 256, 53-</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">58. In my essay I twice misspelled Susanne Pelger's name, for which </span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">I apologize.]</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></em></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">Given nothing more than time and chance, a "light-sensitive patch," they affirmed, <em>can </em>"gradually turn </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">into a focused-lens eye," and in the space of only a </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">few hundred thousand years—a mere moment, as such things go.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">Nilsson and Pelger's paper has, for understand­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">able reasons, been widely circulated and widely </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">praised, and in the literature of evolutionary biolo­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">gy it is now regularly cited as definitive. Not the </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">least of its remarkable authority is derived from the belief that it contains, in the words of one of its de­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">fenders, a "computer simulation of the eye's evolu­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">tion."</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">If this were true, it would provide an extremely <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">important defense of </span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Darwin</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">'s theory. Although a </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">computer simulation is not by itself conclusive—a simulation is one thing, reality another—it is often <span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">an important link in an inferential chain. In the </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">case of </span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">Darwin</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">'s theory, the matter is especially pressing since in the nature of things the theory </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">cannot be confirmed over geological time by any </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">experimental procedure, and it has proved very dif­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">ficult to confirm under laboratory conditions. The </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">claim that the eye's evolution has been successfully </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">simulated by means of Darwinian principles, with </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">results falling well within time scales required by </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">the theory, is thus a matter of exceptional scientific </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">importance.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.55pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">And not just <em>scientific </em>importance, I might add; so </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">dramatic a confirmation of Darwinian theory car­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">ries large implications for our understanding of the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.35pt">human species and its origins. This is no doubt</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"> <span style="color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">why the story of Nilsson and Pelger's computer </span><span style="color: black">simulation has spread throughout the world. Their <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">study has been cited in essays, textbooks, and pop­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">ular treatments of Darwinism <em>like River Out of Eden </em></span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">by the famous Oxford evolutionist Richard Dawkins; accounts of it have made their way onto the Internet in several languages; it has been promot­ed to the status of a certainty and reported as fact </span>in the press, where it is inevitably used to champi­on and vindicate Darwin's theory of evolution.</span></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">In my essay, I suggested that Nilsson and Pelger's </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">arguments are trivial and their conclusions unsub­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">stantiated. I also claimed that representations of </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">their paper by the scientific community have in­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">volved a serious, indeed a flagrant, distortion of their </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">work. But in a letter published in the March issue of </span><span style="font-variant: small-caps">commentary, </span>the physicist Matt Young, whom I <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">singled out for criticism (and whose words I have </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">quoted here), repeated and defended his characteri­zation of Nilsson and Pelger's work as a "computer </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">simulation of the eye's evolution." It is therefore </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">necessary to set the matter straight in some detail.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">I hope this exercise will help to reveal, with a certain uncomfortable clarity, just how scientific orthodoxy works, and how it imposes its opinions on the faithful.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt">Here in their own words is the main argument of Nilsson and Pelger's paper:</span></strong></p> <p style="margin-right: 95.7pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">Theoretical considerations of eye design allow us to find routes along which the optical struc­ture of the</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"> eye may have evolved. If selection constantly favors an increase in the amount of <span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">detectable spatial information, a light-sensitive </span>patch will gradually turn into a focused-lens <span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">eye through continuous small improvements in </span>design. An upper limit for the number of gen­erations required for the complete transforma­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">tion can be calculated with a minimum number </span>of assumptions. Even </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">with a consistently pes­simistic approach, the time required becomes amazingly short: only a few hundred thousand years.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 95.7pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">And here is how they arrived at their conclusions. The setting is "a single circular patch of light-</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">sensi­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">tive cells"—a retina, in effect—"which is bracketed </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.25pt">and surrounded by dark pigment." A "protective lay­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">er" lies above these light-sensitive cells, so that the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">pigment, the light-sensitive cells, and the protective </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.25pt">layer form a kind of sandwich. Concerning the light-</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">sensitive patch itself, Nilsson and Pelger provide no </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">farther details, indicating neither its size nor the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">number of cells it might contain.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.55pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">What they do assume, if only implicitly, is that </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">changes to the initial patch involve either a defor­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">mation of its shape or a thickening of its cells. The </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">patch can be stretched, dimpled, and pulled or </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.25pt">pushed around, and cells may move closer to one an­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">other, like bond salesmen converging on a customer.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.75pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">So much for what changes. What is the change </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">worth? Assuming (reasonably enough) that an eye <span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">is an organ used in order to see, Nilsson and Pel­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">ger represent its value to an organism by a single </span>quantitative character or function, which they des­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">ignate as "spatial resolution" or "visual acuity"— sharp sight, in short. Visual acuity confers an ad­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">vantage on an organism, and so, in any generation, </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">natural selection "constantly favors an increase in the amount of detectable spatial information."</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">There are two ways in which visual acuity may </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">be increased in an initial light-sensitive patch: a) by </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">the "invagination" of the patch, so that it becomes <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">progressively more concave and eventually forms </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">the enclosed interior of a sphere; and b) by the </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">constriction of the sphere's aperture (the two </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">rounded boundaries formed as the flat patch un­</span>dergoes invagination). These changes may be rep­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">resented on sheets of high-school graph paper on </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">which two straight lines—the <em>x </em>and <em>y </em>axes of the </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">system—have been crossed. On the first sheet, rep­resenting invagination, visual acuity moves upward </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">on one axis as invagination moves to the right on </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">the other; on the second sheet, visual acuity moves </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">upward as constriction moves to the right. The </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">curves that </span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt">result, Nilsson and Pelger assert, are continuous and increasing. They do not hurdle over any gaps, and they go steadily upward until they reach a theoretical maximum.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">The similar shape of the two graphs notwith­standing, invagination and aperture constriction exercise different effects on visual acuity. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">"Initially, deepening of the pit" - i.e., invagination - "is by far the most efficient strategy," Nilsson and Pelger write; "but when the pit depth equals the width, aperture constriction becomes more efficient than continued deepening of the pit." From this, they conclude that natural selection would act "first to favor depression and invagination of the light-sen­sitive patch, and then gradually change to favor constriction of the aperture."</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">The result is a pin-hole eye, which is surely an improvement on no eye at all. But there ex­ists an aperture size beyond which visual acuity can­not be improved without the introduction of a lens. Having done all that it can do, the pin-hole eye laps­es. Cells within the light-sensitive sphere now obhg-ingly begin to thicken themselves, bringing about a "local increase" in the eye's refractive index and so forming a lens. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">When die focal length of the lens is 2.55 times its radius—the so-called Mattiessen ratio—the eye will have achieved, Nilsson and Pel-ger write, the "ideal solution for a graded-index lens with a central refractive index of 1.52.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">Thereafter, the lens "changes its shape from el­lipsoid to spherical and moves to the center of cur­vature of the retina." A flat iris "gradually forms by stretching of the original aperture," while the "focal length of the lens . . . gradually shortens, [until] it equals the distance to the</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.05pt"> retina . . . pro­ducing a sharply focused image." The appearance </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">of this spherical, graded-index lens, when placed in </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">the center of curvature of the retina, produces "vir­tually aberration-free imaging over die full 180 de­grees of the visual field."</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">The same assumptions that governed invagina</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">tion and aperture constriction hold sway here as </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">well. Plotted against increasing lens formation, vi­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">sual acuity moves smoothly and steadily upward as </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">a graded-index lens makes its appearance, changes </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">its shape, and moves to center stage. When these </span>transformations have been completed, the result is <span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">a "focused camera-type eye with the geometry typ­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">ical for aquatic animals."</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.95pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">One step remains. Nilsson and Pelger now amal­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">gamate imagination, constriction, and lens form­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">ation into a single "transformation," which they </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">represent by juxtaposing, against changes in visual </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">acuity, changes to the original patch in increments <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">of 1 percent. The resulting curve, specifying quan­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">titatively how much visual acuity may be purchased </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">for each 1-percent unit of change, is ascending, in­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">creasing, and straight, rising like an arrow at an </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">angle of roughly 45 degrees from its point of ori­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">gin. Transformations are "optimal" in the sense </span>that they bring about as much visual acuity as the­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">oretically possible, with the "geometry of each </span>stage [setting] an upper limit to the spatial resolu­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">tion of the eye."</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">It is the existence and shape of this fourth curve that justify their claim that "a light-sensitive patch will gradually turn into a focused-lens eye through <span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">continuous small improvements in <em>design" </em>(empha­</span>sis </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">added). This is not the happiest formulation they could have chosen.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">How much does the initial light-sensitive patch have to change in order to realize a focused camera-<span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">type eye? And how long will it take </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">to do so? These are die questions now before us. </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">As I have mentioned, Nilsson and Pelger assume</span></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.75pt 0.0001pt 2.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">that their initial light-sensitive patch changes in </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">1-percent steps. They illustrate the procedure with <span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">die example of a flat one-foot ruler that also changes </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">in 1-percent steps. After die first step, the ruler will </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">be one foot plus 1 percent of one foot long; after </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">the second step, it will be 1-percent longer than the </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">length just achieved; and so forth. It requires rough­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">ly 70 steps to double a one-foot ruler in length. </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">Putting the matter into symbols, 1.01<sup>70</sup> = 2.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 2.4pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Nilsson and Pelger undertake a very similar cal­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">culation with respect to their initial light-sensitive </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">patch. But since the patch is a three-dimensional </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">object, they are obliged to deal with three dimen­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">sions of change. Growing in steps of 1 percent, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">their <em>blob</em> increases its length, its curvature, and its </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">volume. When all of these changes are shoe-</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">horned together, the patch will have increased in </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">magnitude along some overall (but unspecified) di­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">mension.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.75pt 0.0001pt 2.85pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.5pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">The chief claim of their paper now follows: to </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">achieve the visual acuity that is characteristic of a "focused camera-type eye with the geometry typi­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">cal for aquatic animals," it is necessary that an ini­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">tial patch be made 80,129,540 times larger (or </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">greater or grander) than it originally was. This </span>number represents the <em>magnitude </em>of the blob's in­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">crease in size. How many <em>steps </em>does that figure rep­resent? Since 80,129,540 = 1.01<sup>1.829</sup>, Nilsson and </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">Pelger conclude that "altogether 1,829 steps of 1 percent are required" to bring about the requisite </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">transformation.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 2.9pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.55pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">These steps, it is important to remember, do not </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">represent <em>temporal </em>intervals. We still need to assess </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">how rapidly the advantages represented by such a </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">transformation would spread in a population of or­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">ganisms, and so answer the question of how long the process takes. In order to do this, Nilsson and </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Pelger turn to population genetics. The equation </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">that follows involves the multiplication of four </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">numbers:</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><strong><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt"><span>            </span>R =h<sup>2</sup><span style="font-variant: small-caps"> </span></span></em></strong><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">x <em>i </em>x <em>V x</em></span></strong><strong><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black"> m</span></em></strong><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></strong></p> <p style="margin: 3.6pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0cm; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 13.9pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Here, <em>R </em>is the response (i.e. visual acuity in each </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">generation), <em>h </em>is the coefficient of heredity, <em>i </em>des­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">ignates the intensity of selection, <em>V is </em>the coeffi­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">cient of variation (the ratio of the standard devia­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">tion to the mean), and <em>m, </em>the mean value for visual </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">acuity. These four numbers designate the extent to which heredity is responsible for visual acuity, the <span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">intensity with which selection acts to prize it, the </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">way its mean or average value is spread over a popu</span>lation, and the mean or average value itself. <span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">-</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"> <em>[<span style="color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">A graded-index lens is a lens that is not" optically homogeneous; </span><span style="color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">die figure of 1.52 is "the value close to the upper limit for biologi­</span><span style="color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">cal material."]</span></em><span style="color: black">Val­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">ues are assigned as estimates to the first three </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">numbers; the mean is left undetermined, rising </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">through each generation.</span></span></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">As for the estimates themselves, Nilsson and Pel­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.45pt">ger assume that h<sup>2</sup> = .50; that i = 0.01; and that V= 0.01. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">On this basis, they conclude that <em>R = 0.00005m. </em><strong>The response in each new generation of light-sen­</strong></span><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">sitive patches is 0.00005 times the mean value of </span></strong><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">visual acuity in the previous generation of light-</span></strong><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">sensitive patches.</span></strong><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></strong></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.75pt 0.0001pt 0.3pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.75pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Their overall estimate—the conclusion of their paper—now follows in two stages. Assume that <em>n </em></span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">represents the number of generations required to </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">transform a light-sensitive patch into a "focused </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">camera-type eye with the geometry typical for </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">aquatic animals." (In small aquatic animals, a gen­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.3pt">eration is roughly a year.) If, as we have seen, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">the mean value of visual acuity of such an eye is </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">l.0l<sup>1.829<span>  </span></sup>=80,129,540, where 1,829 represents the <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">number of steps required and 80,129,540 describes </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">the extent of the change those steps bring about; </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.25pt">and if 1.00005<sup>n</sup> = 1.01<sup>1.829</sup> = 80,129,540, then it fol­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">lows that <em>n </em>= 363,992.</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt"></span></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">It is this figure - 363,992 - that allows Nilsson </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">and Pelger to conclude at last that "the time re­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">quired [is] amazingly short: only a few hundred </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">thousand years." And this also completes my expo­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt">sition of Nilsson and Pelger's paper. Business be­fore pleasure.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt"><span>            </span>Nilsson and Pelger's work is a critic's smor­gasbord. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">Questions are free and there are second helpings.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Every scientific paper must begin somewhere....</span><em><span style="font-size: 12pt"><span> </span></span></em></p> <span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt"> </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">...Nilsson and Pelger begin with their assumption </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">that, with respect to the eye, morphological change </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">comes about by imagination, aperture constriction, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">and lens formation. Specialists may wish to know </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">where those light-sensitive cells came from and </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">why there are no other biological structures coor­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">dinated with or contained within the interior of the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">initial patch—for example, blood vessels, nerves, or </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">bones. But these issues may be sensibly deferred.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.95pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Not so the issues that remain. Nilsson and Pel­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">ger treat a biological organ as a physical system, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">one that is subject to the laws of theoretical optics. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">There is nothing amiss in that. But while theoreti­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">cal optics justifies a <em>qualitative </em>relationship between </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">visual acuity on the one hand and invagination, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">aperture constriction, and lens formation on the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">other, the relationships that Nilsson and Pelger </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">specify are tightly <em>quantitative. </em>Numbers make an <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">appearance in each of their graphs: the result, it is</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">claimed, of certain elaborate calculations. But no </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">details are given either in their paper or in its bibli­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">ography. The calculations to which they allude re­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">main out of sight, if not out of mind.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">The 1-percent steps: in what units are they ex­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">pressed? And how much biological change is rep­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">resented by each step? Nilsson and Pelger do not </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">say. Nor do they coordinate morphological change, </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">which they treat as simple, with biochemical change, which in the case of light sensitivity is </span>known to be monstrously complex.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.3pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Does invagination represent a process in which </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">the patch changes as a whole, like a balloon being <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">dimpled, or is it the result of various local process­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">es going off independently as light-sensitive cells jostle with one another and change their position? Are the original light-sensitive cells the complete package, or are new light-sensitive cells added to </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">the ensemble as time proceeds? Do some cells lose </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">their sensitivity and get out of the light-sensing </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">business altogether? We do not know, because </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Nilsson and Pelger do not say.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Biologists commenting on </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Darwin</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">'s theory have </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">almost always assumed that evolution reflects what </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">the French biologist Francois Jacob called <em>brico</em></span><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">lage</span></em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">—a process of tinkering. Biological structures </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.3pt">are put together out of pieces; they adapt their </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">function to changes in their circumstances; they get </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">by. This suggests that in the case of eye formation, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">morphological change might well purchases <em>less </em>vi­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">sual acuity than Nilsson and Pelger assume, the eye </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">being tinkered into existence instead of flogged up an adaptive peak. But if, say, only half as much vi­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">sual acuity is purchased for each of Nilsson and </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">Pelger's 1-percent steps, twice as many steps will be </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">needed to achieve the effect they claim. What is </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">their justification for the remarkably strong asser­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">tion that morphological transformations purchase </span>an optimal amount of visual acuity at each step?</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 11.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">Again we do not know, because they do not say.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">More questions—and we have not even finished </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">the hors d'oeuvres. The plausibility of Nilsson and <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Pelger's paper rests on a single number: 1,829. But </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">without knowing precisely how the number 1,829 has been derived, the reader has no way of deter­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">mining whether it is reasonable or even meaningful.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.95pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.55pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">If nothing else, the number 1,829 represents the <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">maximum point of a curve juxtaposing visual acu­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">ity against morphological transformation. Now, a </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">respect for the ordinary mathematical decencies </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">would suggest that the curve is derived from the </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">number, and the number from various calculations. </span>But all such calculations are missing from Nilsson <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">and Pelger's paper. And if the calculations are not</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"> <span style="color: black">given, neither are any data. Have Nilsson and Pel­ger, for example, <em>verified </em>their estimate, either by <span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">showing that 1,829 1-percent steps do suffice to </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">transform a patch into an eye, or by showing that </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">such an eye may, in 1,829 1-percent steps, be re­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">solved backward into an initial light-sensitive </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">patch? Once again, we do not know because they </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">do not say.</span></span></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.5pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">Still other questions suggest themselves. Al­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">though natural selection is mentioned by Nilsson and Pelger, it is a force that plays no role in their </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">reasoning. Beyond saying that it "constantly favors </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">an increase in the amount of detectable spatial in­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">formation," they say nothing at all. This is an ig­</span>nominious omission in a paper defending Darwin­ian principles. An improvement in visual acuity is no doubt a fine thing for an organism; but no form <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">of biological change is without cost.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.3pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Let us agree that in the development of an eye, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">an initial light-sensitive patch in a given organism becomes invaginated over time. Such a change re­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">quires a corresponding structural change to the or­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">ganism's anatomy. If nothing else, the development of an eye requires the formation of an eye socket— </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">hardly a minor matter in biological terms. Is it re­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">ally the case that an organism otherwise adapted to </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">its environment would discover that the costs in­</span>volved in the reconstruction of its skull are nicely <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">balanced by what would initially be a very modest </span>improvement in sensitivity to light? I can imagine <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">the argument going either way, but surely an argu­ment is needed.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.35pt">Then there is Nilsson and Pelger's data-free </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">way with statistics. What is the basis of the mathe­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">matical values chosen for the numbers they use in assessing how rapidly transformation spreads in a </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">population of eye patches? The coefficient of vari­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">ation is the ratio of the standard deviation to the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">mean. The standard deviation, one might ask, of <em>what? </em>No population figures are given; there are </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">no quantitative estimates of any relevant numeri­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">cal parameter. Why is selection pressure held con­stant over the course of 300,000 years or so, when <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">plainly the advantages to an organism of increas­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">ing light sensitivity will change at every step up the </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">adaptive slope? Why do they call their estimates </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">pessimistic (that is, conservative) rather than wild­ly optimistic?</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Finally, Nilsson and Pelger offer an estimate of </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">the number <em>of steps, </em>computed in 1-percent (actu­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">ally, 1.00005-percent) intervals, that are required to </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">transform their initial patch. At one point, they </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">convert the steps into generations. But a step is not </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">a temporal unit, and, for all anyone knows, each </span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">step could well require half again or twice the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">number of generations they suggest. Why do Nils-<span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">son </span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt">and Pelger match steps to generations in the way they do? I have no idea, and they do not say.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">We are at last at the main course. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">Curiously enough, it is the intellectual demands im­posed by Darwin's</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.1pt"> theory of evolution that serve to </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">empty Nilsson and Pelger's claims of their remain­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">ing plausibility.</span></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Nilsson and Pelger assert that only 363,992 gen­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">erations are required to generate an eye from an </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">initial light-sensitive patch. As I have already ob­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.25pt">served, the number 363,992 is derived from the num­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">ber 80,129,540, which is derived from the number <span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">1,829 - <strong>which in turn is derived from nothing at </strong></span><strong><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">all</span></strong><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">. Nevermind. Let us accept <em>1,829 pour le sport. </em>If </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">Nilsson and Pelger intend their model to be a vin­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">dication of Darwin's theory, then changes from one </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">step to another must be governed by random </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">changes in die model's geometry, followed by some </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">mechanism standing in for natural selection. These are, after all, the crucial features of <em>any </em>Darwinian </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.35pt">theory. But in their paper there is no mention </span><em><span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">whatsoever </span></em><span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">of randomly occurring changes, and nat­</span>ural selection plays only a ceremonial role in their <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">deliberations.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">At the beginning of their paper, Nilsson and Pel­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">ger write of their initial light-sensitive patch that </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">"we <em>expose </em>this structure to selection pressure fa­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">voring spatial resolution" (emphasis added), and </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">later that "[a]s the lens approaches focused condi­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.35pt">tions, <em>selection pressure </em>gradually appears to </span><span style="letter-spacing: 1.95pt">...</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.35pt"> ad­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">just its size to agree with Mattiessen's ratio" (em­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">phasis added). But whatever Nilsson and Pelger </span>may have been doing to their patch, they have not <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">been exposing it to "selection pressure." The patch </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">does only what they have told it to do. By the same </span>token, selection pressures play no role in adjusting <span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">the size of their lenses to agree with Mattiessen's </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">ratio. That agreement is guaranteed, since it is </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Nilsson and Pelger who bring it about, drawing die </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">curve and establishing the relevant results. What </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Nilsson and Pelger <em>assume </em>is that natural selection </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">would track their results; but this assumption is </span>never defended in their paper, nor does it play die slightest role in their theory.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">And for an obvious reason: if there are no random </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">variations occurring in their initial light-sensitive </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">patch, then natural selection has nothing to do. And </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">there are no random variations in that patch, their </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">model succeeding as a defense of </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">Darwin</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">'s theory only by first emptying the theory of its content.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 11.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">An example may make clearer both the point</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"> <span style="color: black">and its importance. Only two steps are required to <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">change the English word "at" to the English word </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">"do": <em>"at" to "ao" and "ao" to "do."</em> The changes </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">are obvious: they have been <em>designed </em>to achieve the </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">specified effect. But such design is forbidden in </span>Darwinian theory. So let us say instead, as </span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">Darwin</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black"> <span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">must, that letters are chosen randomly, for instance </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">by being fished from an urn. In that case, it will </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">take, on average, <em>676 </em>changes (26 letters times 26) </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">to bring about the same two steps.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.5pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">Similarly, depending on assessments of probabili­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">ty, the number of changes required to bring about a </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">single step in Nilsson and Pelger's theory may range </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">widely. It may, in fact, be anything at all. How long </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">would it take to transform a light-sensitive patch </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">into a fully functioning eye? It all depends. It all de­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.25pt">pends on how <em>likely </em>each morphological change hap­pens to be. If cells in their initial light-sensitive patch </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">must discover their appointed role by chance, all es­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">timates of the time required to bring about just the </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">transformations their theory demands—invagina</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">tion, aperture construction, and lens formation— </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">will increase by orders of magnitude.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">If </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">Darwin</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"> were restored to pride of place in Nils<span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">son and Pelger's work, the brief moment involved in </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">their </span>gravamen of my complaints and the dessert of this discussion.</span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">A computer simulation of an evolutionary pro­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">cess is not a mysterious matter. A theory is given, <span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">most often in ordinary mathematical language. The theory's elements are then mapped to ele­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">ments that a computer can recognize, and its dy­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">namical laws, or laws of change, are replicated at a </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">distance by a program. When the computer has </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">run the program, it has simulated the theory.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.95pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.55pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Although easy to grasp as a concept, a computer </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">simulation must meet certain nontrivial require­ments. The computer is a harsh taskmaster, and </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">programming demands a degree of specificity not <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">ordinarily required of a mathematical theory. The </span>great virtue of a computer simulation is that if the set of objects is large, and die probability distribu­tion and fitness function complicated, die comput­er is capable of illustrating the implications of die <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">theory in a way that would be impossible using or­</span>dinary methods of calculation. "Hand calculations <span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">may be sufficient for very simple models," as </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.35pt">Robert E. Keen and James Spain write in their </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">standard text, <strong><em>Computer Simulation</em></strong><em> in Biology </em>(1992), </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">"but computer simulation is almost essential for </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">understanding multi-component models and their </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">complex interrelationships."</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">Whatever the merits of computer simulation, </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">however, they are beside the point in assessing </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Nilsson and Pelger's work. In its six pages, their </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">paper contains no mention of the words "comput­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">er" or "simulation." There are no footnotes indi­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">cating that a computer simulation of their work ex­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">ists, and their bibliography makes no reference to </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">any work containing such a simulation.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.3pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.3pt">Curious about this point, I wrote to Dan-Erik </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Nilsson in the late summer of 2001. "Dear David," </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">he wrote back courteously and at once,</span><sup><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black"></span></sup></p> <p style="background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; margin-right: 88.6pt; text-indent: 11.3pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">You are right that my article with Pelger is not <span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">based on computer simulation of eye evolu­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">tion. I do not know of anyone else who [has] </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">successfully tried to make such a simulation ei­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">ther. But we are currently working on it. To </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">make it behave like real evolution is not a sim­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">ple task. At present our model does produce </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">eyes gradually on the screen, but it does not </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">look pretty, and die genetic algorithms need a fair amount of work before the model will be useful. But we are working on it, and it looks </span>both promising and exciting.</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></em></p> <p style="margin: 6pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0cm; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">These are explicit words, and they are the words </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">of the paper's senior author. I urge readers to keep them in mind as we return to the luckless physicist <span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">Matt Young. In my <span style="font-variant: small-caps">commentary </span>essay of last </span>December, I quoted these remarks by Mr. Young:</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 6pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 12.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Creationists used to argue that . . . there was </span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">not enough time for an eye to develop. A com­</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">puter simulation by Dan-Erik Nilsson and Su</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">sanne Pelger gave the lie to that claim.</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></em></p> <p style="margin: 6.25pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">These, too, are forthright words, but as I have just shown, they are false: Nilsson and Pelger's </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">paper contains no computer simulation, and no </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">computer simulation has been forthcoming from them in all the years since its initial publication. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Sheer carelessness, perhaps? But now, in respond­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">ing to my <span style="font-variant: small-caps">commentary </span>article, Matt Young has </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">redoubled his misreading and proportionately aug­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.25pt">mented his indignation. The full text of his re­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">marks appears in last month's <span style="font-variant: small-caps">commentary; </span>here </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">are die relevant passages:</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 5.75pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 12.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">In describing the paper by Nilsson and Pelger..., </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.35pt">I wrote that they had performed a computer simu­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">lation of die development of die eye. I did not <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">write, as Mr. Berlinski suggests, that they used </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">notihing more than random variation and nat­</span>ural selection, and I know of no reference that <span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">says they did.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0.25pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 12.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">. . .The paper by Nilsson and Pelger is a so­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">phisticated simulation that even includes </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.35pt">quantum noise; it is not, contrary to Mr. </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Berlinski's assertion, a back-of-the-envelope calculation. It begins with a flat, light-sensi­tive patch, which they allow to become con­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">cave in increments of 1 percent, calculating </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">the visual acuity along the way. When some other mechanism will improve acuity faster, </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">they allow, at various stages, the formation of a graded-index lens and an iris, and then opti­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">mize the focus. Unless Nilsson and Pelger </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">performed the calculations in closed form or </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">by hand, theirs was, as I wrote, a "computer </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.3pt">simulation." Computer-aided simulation might </span>have been a slightly better description, but not <span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">enough to justify Mr. Berlinski's sarcasm at my expense. . . .</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 6pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0cm; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">And here is my familiar refrain: <strong>there is <em>no </em>simu­</strong></span><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">lation,</span></strong><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt"> "sophisticated" or otherwise, in Nilsson and </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">Pelger's paper, and their work rests on no such sim­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">ulation; on this point, Nilsson and I are in com­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">plete agreement. Moreover, Nilsson and Pelger do <em><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">not </span></em><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">calculate die visual acuity of any structure, and </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">certainly not over die full 1,829 steps of dieir se­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">quence. They suggest that various calculations have </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">been made, but they do not show how they were </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">made or tell us where diey might be found. At die very best, they have made such calculations for a </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">handful of data points, and then joined those points </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">by a continuous curve.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">There are two equations in Nilsson and Pelger's <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">paper, and neither requires a computer for its solu­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">tion; and <em>there are no others. </em>Using procedures very much like Nilsson and Pelger's own, Mr. Young has </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">nevertheless deduced the existence of a missing </span>computer simulation on theoretical grounds: "Un­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">less Nilsson and Pelger performed the calculations </span>in closed form or by hand, theirs was, as I wrote, a <span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">computer simulation." But another possibility at once suggests itself: that Nilsson and Pelger did </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">not require a computer simulation to undertake </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">their calculations because they made no such cal­culations, their figure of 1,829 steps representing </span>an overall guess based on die known optical char­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">acteristics of existing aquatic eyes.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">Whatever die truth - and I do not know it - Mr. <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Young's inference is pointless. One judges a paper </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">by what it contains and one trusts an author by </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">what he says. No doubt, Matt Young is correct to </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">observe that <em>"computer-aided </em>simulation might </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">have been a better description" of Nilsson and Pel­</span>ger's work. I suppose one could say that had Dan-<span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Erik Nilsson and Susanne Pelger rested their heads</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">on a computer console while trying to guess at the <span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">number of steps involved in transforming a light-</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">sensitive patch into a fully functioning eyeball, </span>their work could also be represented as computer-<span style="letter-spacing: -0.25pt">aided.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt"><span>            </span><span style="font-variant: small-caps; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Matt young </span><span style="color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">is hardly alone in his lavish mis</span><span style="color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">readings. The mathematician Ian Stewart, </span><span style="color: black">who should certainly know better, has made virtu­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">ally the same patently false claims in <em>Nature's Num­</em></span><em><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">bers </span></em><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">(1995). So have many other prominent fig­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">ures. But misreadings are one thing, misrepresen­tations another. More than anyone else, it has been Richard Dawkins who has been responsible for ac­</span>tively misrepresenting Nilsson and Pelger's work, and for disseminating worldwide the notion diat it offers a triumphant vindication of Darwinian prin­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.3pt">ciples.</span></span></span></p> <p style="margin: 2.65pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 1.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">In a chapter of his 1995 book, <em>River Out of Eden, </em></span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">Dawkins writes warmly and at length about Nils-</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">son and Pelger's research. <em>(</em></span><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">A version of the same material by Dawkins, "Where D'you Get<br /> <span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Those Peepers," was published in the <span>New Statesman </span>(</span></span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">July 16, </span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.4pt">1995</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.4pt">).</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></em></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.55pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">Here is what he says </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">(emphasis added throughout):</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 5.75pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 12.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">[Their] task was to set up <em>computer models </em>of </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.25pt">evolving eyes to answer two questions </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.85pt">...[:]</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.25pt"> is </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">there a smooth gradient of change, from flat </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">skin to full camera eye, such that every inter­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">mediate is an improvement? . . <span> </span>[and] how long would the necessary quantity of evolu­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">tionary change take?</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 2.4pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 12.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.05pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">In their <em>computer models, </em>Nilsson and Pelger </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.3pt">made 110 attempts to simulate the internal </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">workings of cells.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 2.4pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 12.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.75pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">. . . Nilsson and Pelger began with a flat </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">retina atop a flat pigment layer and surmount­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.1pt">ed by a flat, protective transparent layer. The </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.2pt">transparent layer was allowed <em>to undergo local­</em></span><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">ized random mutations of its refractive index. </span></em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.15pt">They then let <em>the model transform itself at ran­</em></span><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">dom, </span></em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.05pt">constrained only by the requirement that </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">any change must be small and must be an im­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">provement on what went before.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt">The results were swift and decisive. A trajec­tory of steadily mounting acuity led unhesitat­ingly from the flat beginning through a shal­low indentation to a steadily deepening cup, as the shape of the model eye deformed itself on the computer screen. . . . And then, almost like a con­juring trick, a portion of this transparent filling condensed into a local, spherical region of higher refractive index.</span></p> <p style="margin: 2.9pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 12.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.5pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.2pt">. . . This ratio is called Mattiessen's ratio. </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.25pt">Nilsson and Pelger's <em>computer-simulation model </em></span><em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">homed in </span></em><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">unerringly on Mattiessen's ratio.</span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 5.75pt 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0cm; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 11.3pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt">How very remarkable all this is—inasmuch as </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">there are no computer models mentioned, cited, or contained in Nilsson and Pelger's paper; inasmuch <span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">as Dan-Erik Nilsson denies having based his work on any computer simulations; inasmuch as Nilsson and Pelger never state that their task was to "set up computer models of evolving eyes" for any reason </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">whatsoever; inasmuch as Nilsson and Pelger as­</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">sume but do not prove the existence of "a smooth </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">gradient of change, from flat skin to full camera eye, such that every intermediate is an improve­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">ment"; and inasmuch as the original light-sensitive </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">patch in Nilsson and Pelger's paper was never al­lowed to undergo "localized random mutations of </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">its refractive index."</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.5pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">And how very remarkable again—inasmuch as <span style="letter-spacing: 0.3pt">there are no computer "screens" mentioned or cited by Nilsson and Pelger, no indication that </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">their illustrations were computer-generated, and </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">no evidence that they ever provided anyone with a </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.25pt">real-time simulation of their paper where one </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">could observe, "almost like a conjuring trick," the "swift and decisive" results of a process that they </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">also happen to have designed.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.25pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.8pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">And yet again how very remarkable—inasmuch </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.35pt">as Nilsson and Pelger's "computer-simulation </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">model" did not home in unerringly on Mattiessen's </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">ratio, Nilsson and Pelger having done all the hom­<span style="letter-spacing: 0.2pt">ing themselves and thus sparing their model the </span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.05pt">trouble.</span></span><span style="font-size: 12pt"></span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.05pt">Each and every one of these very remarkable as­</span><span style="font-size: 12pt">severations can be explained as the result of care­lessness only if one first </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">indicts their author for gross incompetence.</span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><strong><span style="font-size: 12pt">Final questions</span></strong><span style="font-size: 12pt">. Why, in the nine years since their work appeared, have Nilsson and Pelger never dissociated themselves from c</span><span style="font-size: 12pt; letter-spacing: -0.25pt">laims about their </span><span style="font-size: 12pt">work that they know are unfounded? This may not <span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">exactly be dishonest, but it hardly elicits admiration. </span>More seriously, what of the various masters of in­dignation, those who are usually so quick to de­<span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">nounce critics of Darwin's theory as carrying out the </span>devil's work? Eugenie Scott, Barbara Forrest, <span style="letter-spacing: -0.15pt">Lawrence Krauss, Robert T. Pennock, Philip Kitch</span><span style="letter-spacing: 0.1pt">er, Kelly Smith, Daniel Dennett, Paul Gross, Ken </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.2pt">Miller, Steven Pinker—they are all warm from com­</span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.05pt">bat. Why have they never found reason to bring up the matter of the mammalian eye and the computer </span><span style="letter-spacing: -0.1pt">simulation that does not exist?</span></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.3pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: -0.1pt">And what should we call such a state of affairs? I </span><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black">suggest that scientific fraud will do as well as any <span style="letter-spacing: 0.15pt">other term.</span></span></p> <p style="margin: 0cm 88.6pt 0.0001pt 0.7pt; background: white none repeat scroll 0% 50%; text-indent: 10.3pt; -moz-background-clip: -moz-initial; -moz-background-origin: -moz-initial; -moz-background-inline-policy: -moz-initial" class="MsoNormal"><span style="font-size: 12pt; color: black; letter-spacing: 0.15pt"> </span></p> <p style="margin-right: 88.6pt" class="MsoNormal"><em><span style="font-size: 12pt">David Berlinski is the author of A Tour of the Calcu­lus, The Advent of the Algorithm, and </span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt">Newton</span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt">'s Gift. His new book, Secrets of the Vaulted Sky, is forthcoming from Harcourt later this year. </span></em><em><span style="font-size: 12pt">Paper from the <span> </span>Commentary, vol. 115, April 2003, no.4.<span> </span></span></em></p> </td> </tr> </table> <span class="article_seperator"> </span> <table align="center" style="margin-top: 25px;"> <tr> </body></html>